Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Educ., 03 October 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
Volume 7 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/feduc.2022.948650

Exploring achievement-level differences in implementing self-regulated learning instruction in a classical Chinese reading intervention program

  • Department of Curriculum and Instruction, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Shatin, Hong Kong SAR, China

Research on self-regulated learning (SRL) instruction typically treats students as a homogeneous group, without considering the possible effects of students’ prior level of SRL competence on the implementation and effectiveness of SRL instruction. To fill this research gap, this study explored the differences in learning outcomes among students with different achievement levels who received an SRL-based classical Chinese reading instruction program. Sixty seven ninth graders from two classes, one with high achievers and the other with moderate achievers, from one secondary school in Hong Kong participated in an intervention program designed by this study’s researchers. The study adopted qualitative methods, including classroom observations, teacher and student interviews, along with supplementary quantitative methods, including reading test and questionnaire, to explore the differences between the two achiever-groups when the intervention program was implemented. The results showed that the two classes differed on students’ SRL performance, the degree of teacher support and student autonomy, and students’ perceptions of SRL instruction. Implications of the findings for SRL research and intervention design are discussed.

Introduction

Self-regulated learning (SRL) is an active and constructive learning process through which learners set learning goals and accordingly monitor, regulate, and control their cognition, motivation, and behavior toward their goals (Winne and Perry, 2000; Pintrich and Zusho, 2002). With extensive evidence on the positive influence of SRL on both student learning and achievement (Zimmerman, 2001; Broadbent and Poon, 2015), researchers have emphasized the importance of integrating SRL into classroom practices to facilitate essential skill development both in and beyond school (Dignath and Büttner, 2018; Schunk and Greene, 2018). However, most studies on SRL instruction treat students as a homogeneous group, without considering the possible effects of students’ prior level of SRL competence on the implementation and effectiveness of SRL instruction. This study seeks to fill this gap by exploring the differences in learning outcomes among students with different achievement levels when they underwent an SRL-based classical Chinese (CC) reading instruction program. The aim is to generate insights into how SRL interventions can be customized to meet individual student needs.

Self-regulated learning and self-regulated learning instruction

Self-regulated learning is an important area of research in educational psychology (Panadero, 2017). While its definitions vary across different SRL models, most researchers agree that SRL commonly involves three core components–cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational aspects–and three cyclical phases of learning–forethought, performance, and reflection (Zimmerman, 2001; Stoeger et al., 2015; Usher and Schunk, 2018; Greene et al., 2019). Self-regulated learners exercise metacognition by analyzing the demands of tasks, setting appropriate goals to guide their learning, and making critical reflections on their learning process and results. They are also motivated to learn actively and strategically by applying a repertoire of effective strategies to self-monitor their learning process, and optimize their learning performance accordingly (Perry et al., 2007; Dignath and Büttner, 2018).

Many researchers have emphasized the importance of classroom context/characteristics in facilitating or inhibiting students’ development of SRL (Lombaerts et al., 2009; Neitzel and Connor, 2017; Dignath and Büttner, 2018). We can synthesize four major instructional principles from the literature regarding the classroom instruction features that support students in SRL (van Grinsven and Tillema, 2006; Housand and Reis, 2008; Lombaerts et al., 2009; Perry and Rahim, 2011; Stoeger et al., 2015; Neitzel and Connor, 2017; Jayawardena et al., 2019; Mohammadi et al., 2020; Dignath and Veenman, 2021; Cousins et al., 2022): (1) Task nature: Teachers provide explicit learning strategy instruction, and design open, complex, and authentic learning materials and activities to facilitate higher-order thinking skills and learning motivation. (2) Teacher support: Teachers use direct instruction, modeling, coaching, scaffolding, and feedback to support SRL skills learning. (3) Student autonomy: Student-directed activities are designed such that students can practice their SRL skills by gaining control over the learning process. (4) Evaluation practices: Mastery-oriented assessments, and both self- and peer-evaluations are used to enhance metacognition and self-efficacy.

Student achievement-level differences in self-regulated learning

Very few studies have investigated how individual student differences may affect the implementation of SRL instruction (Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016). Studies have indicated that high achievers (HAs) generally have higher SRL competence than low achievers (LAs) in terms of disciplinary knowledge, strategy use, metacognition, self-efficacy, and learning motivation (Broadbent and Poon, 2015; Emagnaw, 2019; Bai et al., 2021). In a think-aloud study, Heirweg et al. (2020) also found clear differences in the quality of students’ SRL processes: HAs showed a more strategic and adaptive approach to learning during all learning processes compared to LAs and average achievers. HAs were more capable of strategically and effectively planning their tasks, combining different cognitive strategies, and adopting self-evaluations to regulate their learning process. Thus, as different achievers have different SRL levels before receiving SRL instruction, they may also have different responses, and thus, display different degrees of improvement after the intervention.

However, there are competing views regarding which achiever groups may benefit the most from SRL instruction. Since SRL is challenging for students, some researchers believe that HAs will benefit more than LAs. This is because HAs are able to control their cognition more efficiently, and thus, learn and apply SRL skills more easily (Hattie et al., 1996; Otto and Kistner, 2017). This hypothesis suggests that teaching SRL skills to students of different achievement levels may exacerbate the Matthew effect: more competent students will gain more from the intervention than their less competent counterparts, thus leading to greater differences and inequalities (Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016). A contrary hypothesis is that LAs will benefit more from SRL instruction by learning and practicing the newly learned SRL skills, whereas HAs with high-baseline SRL levels have little room for improvement (Zohar and Peled, 2008; Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016; Peeters et al., 2016). Researchers who hold this view propose a compensation effect, suggesting that SRL instruction is likely to help LAs develop strategies to compensate for their low cognitive abilities. Studies do find different effects of SRL instruction due to different student achievement levels. Essentially, either HAs (Otto and Kistner, 2017), LAs (Zohar and Peled, 2008), or moderate achievers (MAs) (Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016) benefit the most, or that there are no differential effects for specific achiever groups (Stoeger and Ziegler, 2010; Donker et al., 2014).

The majority of SRL studies on students’ achievement-level differences adopt quantitative designs to compare pre- and post-test measures. However, since students must participate actively and engage in higher-level thinking while receiving SRL instruction (Perry et al., 2002; van Grinsven and Tillema, 2006), their baseline SRL condition should substantially affect both teaching and learning processes during the intervention. For instance, studies exploring teacher-related factors reveal that student achievement levels influence teachers’ willingness to as well as the degree to which they adopt SRL instruction. SRL instruction is more frequently observed among HAs than among LAs (Guo et al., 2019). Teachers do not tend to teach SRL skills to LAs to the same extent as they do to HAs because they believe that LAs are incapable of self-regulating the learning process (Zohar and Peled, 2008; Lau and Chen, 2013; Peeters et al., 2016; Yan, 2018). Some researchers have also noted that LAs require more intensive teacher support and a longer period to develop their SRL skills as they have weak SRL foundations and poor motivation (Zohar and Peled, 2008; Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016). This suggests that achievement-level differences may exist when teachers implement SRL instruction for different achiever groups.

Achievement-level differences in classical Chinese reading instruction

Classical Chinese is the official written language in ancient China (Wang, 1979). Learning to read CC is a core component of the language curriculum in Chinese society. The current methods of teaching and learning CC reading are deeply influenced by traditional Confucian culture. While various student-centered pedagogies have been introduced in recent years, teachers play a leading role in CC reading instruction (Tang and Sun, 2013; Lau, 2017; Song, 2021). Moreover, CC and modern Chinese have many linguistic difference; hence, many students face difficulties when reading CC texts (Chi and Chiou, 2015; Lau, 2018; Ren and Yang, 2019). This makes the students reliant on their teachers. Moreover, due to these reading difficulties, students usually demonstrate very low confidence and lack intrinsic interest in reading CC texts (Lau, 2019; Song, 2021). Hence, the traditional teacher-centered approach, and students’ poor CC reading ability and motivation may increase the difficulty of implementing SRL instruction in teaching CC reading.

Some researchers have suggested that student SRL levels vary owing to the different cognitive demands of each academic subject (Dignath and Büttner, 2008; Virtanen and Nevgi, 2010; Cousins et al., 2022). To overcome the difficulties in CC reading, Chinese scholars have proposed that students must be equipped with sufficient knowledge of the CC language and ancient Chinese culture, and learn how to use various CC reading strategies effectively (Zhao, 2004; Shangguan, 2008; Chi and Chiou, 2015; Lau, 2018). Moreover, considering the difficulty involved in CC reading, students with better knowledge backgrounds and cognitive competences may benefit more if SRL instruction is implemented for CC reading. Thus, achievement-level differences may be more robust.

Purpose of the study

This study was part of a research project titled “The effectiveness of self-regulated learning-based instruction on students’ classical Chinese reading comprehension and motivation.” This project aimed to integrate SRL into CC reading instruction to enhance CC reading competence and motivation among students in Hong Kong. The data used here were mainly drawn from two experimental classes to explore possible achievement-level differences during and after the implementation of the SRL intervention program. Only a few recent studies have examined the differential effects of SRL instruction on students of different achievement levels using quantitative methods; moreover, their results are inconsistent. Hence, this study adopted qualitative methods supplemented by quantitative methods to generate new insights into how SRL interventions can be customized to meet individual student needs. The theoretical framework is illustrated in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The theoretical framework of the study.

The research questions were as follows:

1. What were the differences in SRL performance between the two achievement groups when they underwent the SRL intervention program?

2. What were the differences in teachers’ implementation of SRL instruction between the two achievement groups?

3. What were the differences in the effectiveness of the SRL intervention program between the two achievement groups?

This study addresses several unexplored issues in the SRL literature. First, studies on the SRL instruction implementation process have primarily focused on teaching influences while ignoring the key role of students. This study offers a new perspective on which factors influence the implementation of SRL instruction. Second, most studies regard students as a homogenous group when evaluating the effectiveness of SRL instruction. While a few recent studies have examined the differential effects of SRL instruction on students with different achievement levels, the results have been inconsistent. Third, while most SRL intervention studies have only used quantitative approaches to compare the learning outcomes of different achiever groups, this study adopts both quantitative and qualitative methods. Thus, its findings can generate new insights into how SRL interventions can be customized to meet individual student needs.

Materials and methods

Participants

One secondary school in Hong Kong was invited to implement this study’s intervention program among Grade 9 students. The selection of the experimental school was based on the school’s readiness to adopt a new approach to teaching CC reading. This school is classified as a Band 2 school, which consists mainly of the middle 33.3% of students under the Hong Kong Secondary School Place Allocation System. These students were further allocated to different classes based on their internal examination scores. Two classes from Grade 10 [Class A with high achievers (n = 34) and Class B with moderate achievers (n = 33)] were randomly assigned as an experimental group (EG), whereas, two other classes with equivalent achievement levels were assigned as a control group (CG). Data were drawn from the EG, which contained 67 combined students from two classes (34 boys and 33 girls) aged between 13 and 15 years (mean = 14.02 years, SD = 0.60). The two EG classes did not differ significantly in terms of sex distribution or age. All study participants provided informed consent and the study design was approved by the appropriate ethics review board.

Instruments

Classical Chinese reading comprehension test

A CC reading comprehension test was designed to assess students’ CC reading ability. It consisted of one narrative and one argumentative text. Five “word interpretation” questions and nine “text comprehension” questions were designed for each text. Each test item was scored on 0 to 2 points, with a total score of 56.

Classical Chinese reading questionnaire

The questionnaire consisted of three sections. The first section assessed students’ frequency of using various word- (Zhou, 2007; Wei, 2009) and text-level strategies (Pressley and Afflerbach, 1995; Mokhtari and Reichard, 2002) when reading CC texts. The second section measured students’ familiarity with linguistic (Pulleyblank, 1995; Zhang, 2005) and content knowledge (Xiong, 1993; Zhao, 2004) essential to CC reading. The third section measured students’ self-efficacy, intrinsic motivation, and extrinsic motivation in CC reading (Lau, 2004).

Classroom observations

An adapted observation form was designed based on Perry (1998) and Perry et al. (2007) to ascertain how SRL instruction was implemented during the intervention program. The form contained four sections: the first and second were used to record background information and maintain a running record of classroom events; the third was designed to record information related to the four major SRL instructional principles; and the fourth was used to record students’ SRL-related behaviors and performance.

Semi-structured student group interviews

Each interview included two sets of questions. In the first set, students were asked about their strategy usage, prior knowledge, and motivation to read CC texts during the module. This was designed to understand their learning progress and level of SRL. For the second set, students were asked about their general views and perceptions of the program’s usefulness.

Semi-structured teacher interviews

Each interview included two sets of questions. The first set focused on intervention implementation. Teachers were asked whether they had used all learning materials and activities in the module, as well as their reasons for doing or not doing so. The second set of questions focused on how teachers evaluated students’ SRL performance and the effectiveness of SRL instruction in enhancing student CC.

Procedure

The study lasted for 1 year and was conducted during regular school lessons. Students in both EG and CG had the same number of Chinese language lessons, and read the same set of CC texts during the study. EG teachers implemented SRL instruction in their classes by following the instructional practices specified in an intervention package designed developed this study’s researchers based on the four SRL instructional principles reviewed in the Section “Introduction.” Meanwhile, CG teachers adopted traditional teacher-centered instructional practices to teach the same texts, including lecturing, questioning, and worksheet exercises. The intervention program consisted of two CC reading modules, each involving 14–16 50-min lessons. A CC reading module was arranged in each semester. A brief summary of the intervention design is presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Brief description of the intervention design.

Two workshops were conducted prior to the program to introduce teachers to the intervention design as well as the concepts and principles of SRL instruction. The researchers explained the study’s purpose, data collection procedures, and confidentiality rules to the teachers during the first collaborative meeting. Pretest quantitative measures were collected at the beginning of the first semester. Each experimental class was observed twice (once each during the first and second modules). Teacher interviews were arranged immediately after each classroom observation. Six students from each class with different ability levels were invited to participate in a group interview after each module. The post-test quantitative measures were collected near the end of the second semester.

Results

Differences in students’ self-regulated learning performance during the intervention

The findings of independent samples t-tests indicated that the HAs in Class A scored significantly higher than the MAs in Class B in all pre-test quantitative measures (Table 2). This suggests that before the intervention, HAs had better SRL baseline conditions and CC reading abilities than MAs.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Pre-test comparisons on the two experimental classes.

High achievers also outperformed MAs on different aspects of SRL during the intervention. First, regarding the cognitive aspect of SRL, HAs were more skillful than MAs in applying their newly learned strategies to achieve a more comprehensive understanding of the texts. For example, students learned the questioning strategy in the first module and then applied it during a roleplaying activity to facilitate their comprehension of a CC story. As shown in the following excerpts, HAs were able to ask high-level questions. Further, their answers reflected a comprehensive understanding of character intentions and feelings. Meanwhile, MAs usually asked simple or irrelevant questions and only gave brief answers.

Excerpts from Class A’s first lesson observation are as follows (OCA201811151):

HA1 [questioner]: Stepmother, why is it that you treated your stepson well when your husband was alive but treated him badly after your husband died? Did you want to take revenge on him? Later, you treated him well again. Was it because you want him to treat you well when you grow old?2 [The questions covered all story contents and challenged the role player to explain the character’s intentions in detail.]

HA2 [playing the role of the stepmother]: Of course not. I treated him well because when his father was alive, he was willing to study. When his father passed away, he became lazy and refused to study. I hope this experience will teach him to cherish the opportunity to study. [This answer reflected that HA2 accurately comprehended the implicit meaning of the story.]

Excerpts from Class B’s first lesson observation are as follows (OCB20181115):

MA1 [questioner]: Stepson, which do you like better: feeding the pigs or going to school? [Simple “yes/no” question].

MA2 [playing the role of the stepson]: Feeding the pigs. [This answer contradicted the meaning of the story].

MA3 [questioner]: Servant, why do you keep cleaning the board? [An irrelevant question].

MA4 [playing the role of the servant]: Because I am a servant. [A meaningless answer].

Second, regarding the metacognitive aspect of SRL, HAs had clear ideas about the learning objectives, activity requirements, and standards of good strategy use in the two observed lessons. For example, when students in Class A misinterpreted a CC text, their classmates immediately pointed out the problem without waiting for the teacher’s instruction. The teacher of Class A (Teacher A) also found that her students could make precise self- and peer-evaluations.

TA: After the group presentation, I noticed some unsatisfactory performances. I asked students for their comments and they were able to point out their problems … They were also able to give their peers constructive feedback. (TICA20190530)

Meanwhile, MAs’ performance in the first observed lesson indicated that they did not have clear ideas about the learning goals and learning task requirements. They simply followed their teacher’s instructions, and relied heavily on her guidance and feedback to gauge their performance. Class B’s teacher (Teacher B) said that her students’ responses to the self-reflection form were very simple, indicating poor metacognitive ability.

Third, regarding the motivational aspect of SRL, HAs showed more confidence than MAs while participating in class activities. For example, while most HAs asked fluent questions and promptly responded to their classmates’ questions, most MAs read questions with uncertainty and always hesitated to provide answers. During the interviews, some MAs said they would avoid reading CC texts, while some HAs said they regarded CC reading as a platform for demonstrating their abilities.

MA4: I want to avoid reading CC texts as I find them very difficult. (SICB20181210)

HA3: I like CC reading … because if other students fail to get the meaning but I can get it, I will have a sense of achievement. (SICA20181210)

High achievers also had higher levels of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for CC reading than MAs. Most MAs said that they disliked CC reading prior to the intervention because of its difficulty.

Some HAs said they liked CC reading for intrinsic reasons, whereas others said they were willing to learn CC texts for extrinsic reasons.

MA6: I don’t like [CC reading] because I do not understand it. (SICB20181210)

HA6: I find CC texts interesting. Although CC words are difficult, I like the stories in the texts. (SICA20181210)

HA1: I’m neutral. If I get better examination scores in CC reading, I will like it more. (SICA20181210)

In the observed lessons for Class A, HAs were always active and willing to interact with their peers. A few HAs demonstrated high degrees of extrinsic motivation by showing off their good comprehension abilities and strategic skills during the observed lessons. Although students in Class B generally enjoyed the activities and displayed involvement during the observed lessons, some intentionally asked irrelevant questions to make their peers laugh, whereas others were inattentive.

Differences in teachers’ implementation of self-regulated learning instruction

During interviews, both teachers and students in the two classes reported that before the study, traditional teacher-centered instruction was the major instructional approach to teaching CC texts and that the teachers made obvious changes when implementing the intervention program. The observed lessons entailed a similar nature of task because both teachers followed the intervention package to deliver their lessons. However, there were notable differences between the two classes regarding the teachers’ and students’ roles. Class A generally demonstrated a high degree of student autonomy and low degree of teacher support. These students played the dominant role in most activities. They had a proficient grasp of the CC texts and activity procedures, actively participated in activities, and commented on peer performance. Teacher A said that before participating in the project, she was worried about how her students would adapt to the new instructional approach. Their good performance was beyond her expectations. This made her believe that they could develop into self-regulated learners, thereby encouraging her to give them a higher degree of autonomy.

TA: Their performance was beyond what I had expected. Most of them were well-prepared and involved in the activity. … The most impressive change I made was being able to release my hold of the classroom. In this lesson, I only talked for about 5–10 min. Through interactions with their peers, students inspired each other to think deeper. I gradually realized that both students and I were readers [of the same level]. They also inspired me a lot. (TICA20181115)

The situation in Class B was different. As mentioned above, the MAs in Class B only had a rough understanding of the CC texts, used surface-level reading strategies, lacked confidence, and tended to play rather than learn seriously during class activities. Furthermore, a high degree of teacher support was observed in Class B. Teacher B used various scaffolding strategies to engage her students in meaningful learning, including giving detailed explanations and clear directions, modeling how to use strategies, providing individualized guidance to different groups, and giving prompts and directive feedback to students when they failed to answer questions or misuse the strategies. The example below illustrates how Teacher B helped her students improve their questioning and answering skills.

Excerpts from Class B’s first lesson observation (OCB20181115):

MA3 [questioner]: Servant, why do you keep cleaning the board? [An irrelevant question].

MA4 [playing the role of the servant]: Because I am a servant. [A meaningless answer].

TB: Good. No one asked the servant anything before. However, you have to ask the servant a meaningful question. [Gave affirmation and direction to the questioner].

MA3: What do you think about how the stepmother has treated her stepson? [A question that required the character to comment on the text content].

MA4: She wanted her son to be good, but he didn’t listen to her. [An answer that showed MA4 understood the story accurately].

TB: Does anyone want to ask a follow-up question? You may try to ask about the feelings of the stepmother and her stepson. [Gave direction and encouraged students to ask deeper questions].

MA5: Stepmother, do you feel distressed by your stepson’s bad behavior? [A question concerning the character’s feelings].

MA1 [playing the role of the stepmother]: Yes, I feel distressed. [A simple answer].

TB: Let’s clap for them. In this round, most of the questions were straightforward and at the surface level. I hope all of you will spend more time thinking of deeper questions. Asking whether the stepmother feels distressed is good. However, try moving beyond simple “yes/no” questions. Think about what else you want to know. [Finished this round of activity by praising her students and giving concrete feedback and directions].

Students in Class B mainly played the role of “followers” during the first observed lesson. Although they were given a certain degree of autonomy because of the open nature of the activities, they tended to wait for Teacher B to give instructions and expected her to play an authoritative role during class. During the student interviews, some MAs clearly expressed that teacher guidance was important because they did not have confidence in their own interpretation of the texts.

Furthermore, Teacher B mentioned the students’ weak abilities several times when discussing their unsatisfactory performance in her two interviews. She explained that she chose to guide students herself rather than letting them discuss the material among themselves because they were not skilled at group discussions. She also eliminated some student-directed activities because she believed that they were too difficult for her students. She noted:

TB: “My class is weak. The debate activity is too difficult for them. … The activity required students to be very familiar with cultural knowledge. That’s why I skipped it.” (TICB20190530)

Differences in the effectiveness of self-regulated learning instruction

Importantly, the pair-samples t-tests indicated that MAs improved significantly on most post-test quantitative measures, including reading test scores, prior knowledge, and intrinsic motivation. Meanwhile, HAs only showed significant improvement in their reading test scores (Table 3). MAs scored significantly lower than HAs on all pre-test measures prior to the intervention. Although their mean scores were still lower than those of HAs in all post-test measures, the findings of one-way ANCOVAs using pre-test scores as covariates indicated no significant differences between the two groups on any post-test measure (Table 4). These findings suggest that the gap between HAs and MAs in their SRL and CC reading abilities narrowed after the program.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pre- and post-test comparisons within each experimental class.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Group comparisons on the post-test measures among the two experimental classes.

During the interviews, both HAs and MAs had positive views on the intervention program. Most agreed that the intervention program was effective in enhancing their CC reading motivation. They liked the interesting learning materials, open tasks, and interactive activities. The task nature of SRL instruction aroused their intrinsic motivation to read CC texts and facilitated their involvement in classroom learning.

HA4: The stories in this module are interesting. After this module, I want to read more stories on this topic. (SICA20181210)

HA1: The activities are interesting. The discussions and debates with our classmates increased our interest in the texts, and we learned more than before. (SICA20190605)

MA3: I am more interested in learning CC reading now because of the many activities. It is different from previous CC reading classes that were always very boring. (SICB20190605)

Further, many MAs mentioned that their CC reading motivation improved because the new strategies increased their self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation toward CC reading. As shown in the following example, students’ CC reading abilities, self-efficacy, and intrinsic motivations were closely related and mutually reinforced through the program:

MA4: My motivation improved. I used to be very afraid of reading CC texts because I did not understand them. Now, since the teacher has used different types of activities to help us learn the texts, I feel that learning CC reading is easier and more interesting. (SICB20190605)

Notably, some HAs said that the program did not affect their motivation because they were already highly motivated to read. This may explain why there was no significant motivational difference among HAs.

HA2: No change. I have always been interested in CC reading and continue to be interested. (SICA20190605)

Interestingly, the perception of HAs and MAs clearly differed on the intervention program’s effectiveness in enhancing their CC reading abilities. During the interviews, all MAs mentioned that their CC reading skills and comprehension were significantly improved after learning the reading strategies or participating in the self-directed activities.

MA1: Yes, the program helped improve my CC reading ability. Previously, I only knew the adding strategy but now I am aware of more strategies to interpret CC words. (SICB20190605)

MA2: In Grade 7 and Grade 8, I just listened to and wrote down my teachers’ explanations of the CC texts. Now, I do not just copy and recite my teacher’s model answers but can understand the texts by myself through participation in the class activities. (SICB20181210)

By contrast, HAs perceived improvements in CC reading in less obvious ways than MAs did. Some HAs said that they did not gain much from the program because they were already familiar with most of the content. Others said that the program did not enhance their reading ability because the teacher spent less time explaining the meanings of the CC words than before.

HA4: Not much [improvement]. I had always used strategies to read CC texts; the module just taught me their names. (SICA20181210)

HA1: This instructional approach can enhance my interest but not my reading ability. When compared to the previous teacher-centered approach, our teacher spent less time on explaining the words of the CC texts to us. So my word-interpretation skills did not improve. (SICA20190605)

Both Teachers A and B observed that students’ CC reading motivation increased during the intervention program. Owing to their students’ different ability levels, Teacher A focused on higher-order thinking, whereas Teacher B was more concerned about basic foundations.

TA: All students were very involved in the activities … The activities were successful in linking student learning to their real lives and stimulated them to think deeper. (TICA20190530)

TB: The program helped my students establish a good foundation for CC reading. Although my class is weak, student performance exceeded my expectations. Many students were willing to participate and use the strategies … Now, I find that the learning atmosphere is better than before. (TICB20181115)

Discussion

This study explored the differences in learning outcomes among students with different achievement levels when they underwent an SRL instruction program. Consistent with research (Broadbent and Poon, 2015; Emagnaw, 2019; Heirweg et al., 2020; Bai et al., 2021), pre-test comparisons indicated that HAs had better baseline SRL conditions than MAs. Moreover, during the program, HAs actively participated in student-directed activities to achieve a deeper understanding of the CC texts. By contrast, MAs required intensive guidance from teachers to complete similar tasks. This finding supports some scholarly views that better baseline SRL competence results in better learning and application of SRL skills (Hattie et al., 1996; Otto and Kistner, 2017), while weak students require more teacher support to adapt to the SRL instruction environment (Zohar and Peled, 2008; Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016). These achievement-level differences in student SRL performance reveal that besides teachers’ perceptions, student SRL levels may influence whether teachers implement this method.

Studies generally find a high degree of teacher support and a low degree of student autonomy for SRL instruction in Chinese classrooms (Lau, 2012; Lau and Chen, 2013). Meanwhile, this study found that the students’ achievement level was an important factor in determining the degree of teacher support and student autonomy. Although both teachers participating in the study were used to traditional teacher-centered instruction when teaching CC texts, different degrees of teacher support and student autonomy were observed in their classes during the intervention program. The interviews indicated that students’ abilities and motivation levels were the major concerns of the two teachers. HAs’ good performance during student-directed activities successfully changed Teacher A’s original perception and increased her willingness to allow students higher degrees of autonomy. However, MAs’ unsatisfactory performance led Teacher B to maintain a more teacher-directed approach in guiding students through learning activities. Indeed, studies on SRL instruction have revealed that student characteristics are major factors affecting teachers’ willingness to implement these types of programs (Peeters et al., 2016). Because SRL is challenging for many students (Schunk and Zimmerman, 1997; Perry et al., 2002), teachers typically decide whether they should use SRL instruction based on students’ knowledge, strategic skills, and motivation (Zohar and Peled, 2008; Lau and Chen, 2013; Peeters et al., 2016; Yan, 2018).

Notably, there have been controversial views regarding which achiever groups benefit more from SRL instruction. Using pre- and post-test comparison methods, Zohar and Peled (2008), Dörrenbächer and Perels (2016), and Otto and Kistner (2017) found that HAs, MAs, and LAs, respectively, benefited the most from SRL instruction. Meanwhile, others find no differential effects for specific achiever groups (Stoeger and Ziegler, 2010; Donker et al., 2014). This study’s findings suggest that MAs benefited more from SRL instruction than HAs. Specifically, MAs improved more than HAs, and the gap between HAs and MAs narrowed on all quantitative measures. Furthermore, while both HAs and MAs had positive perceptions of SRL instruction, more MAs agreed that SRL instruction was effective in enhancing their CC reading motivation and abilities. This may be because HAs already had basic knowledge and skills, and were motivated to read CC texts; hence, they experienced relatively less improvement than MAs, who once regarded CC reading as very difficult. These findings support the hypothesis that SRL instruction is more useful in helping weak students develop strategies to compensate for their low cognitive abilities, whereas, HAs may have little room for improvement (Zohar and Peled, 2008; Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016; Peeters et al., 2016).

The achievement-level differences revealed here suggest that while SRL instruction should benefit students across ability levels (Donker et al., 2014), intervention programs should be customized to individual student needs (Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016). Among the four major principles of SRL instruction, task nature was well-received by both HAs and MAs. This suggests that direct strategy instruction, and open, authentic, and interactive learning tasks facilitate learning regardless of achievement level. For HAs with better knowledge, strategic skills, and metacognitive awareness, higher degrees of student autonomy and evaluation practices are more suitable. The good performance of HAs in strategy use, and self- and peer-evaluation suggests that involving HAs in student-directed activities and assessments should facilitate higher-order thinking, and increase their ownership and sense of achievement in learning. Student-directed activities and assessments also enhance students’ abilities, self-efficacy, and intrinsic and extrinsic motivations (Panadero et al., 2017; Jayawardena et al., 2019). Although many MAs and LAs may not yet have the ability to self-regulate the learning process, teachers should not simply conclude that SRL is unsuitable for them. Rather, a high degree of teacher support should be used to help weak students establish solid foundations during the early stages of SRL instruction. The obvious improvements in the MAs observed here suggest that teachers can gradually increase the degree of student autonomy and evaluation practices when students are ready to self-regulate their learning.

Limitations and future research directions

This study offers several suggestions for further research based on its limitations.

While the findings highlight the importance of exploring students’ individual differences during SRL interventions, this study only focused on achievement-level differences. Other individual differences, such as age and gender, can also influence learning in the SRL context (Pintrich and Zusho, 2002; Dignath and Büttner, 2018; Zhu and Mok, 2018). Hence, future studies should investigate whether these individual differences also affect the implementation and effectiveness of SRL instruction. Understanding the mediating effects of more student factors is useful for developing more tailor-made intervention programs to cater to the needs of different types of students.

Most studies on achievement-level differences compared HAs and LAs (e.g., Dörrenbächer and Perels, 2016; Otto and Kistner, 2017; Bai et al., 2021). However, the participating school in this study only admitted students with high and moderate achievement levels. Thus, no LAs were analyzed. Considering the clear differences between HAs and MAs observed in this study, the achievement-level differences between HAs and LAs is likely to be larger. Thus, future studies should include more students with different achievement levels to explore how differences in students’ achievement may affect the implementation and effectiveness of SRL instruction. Contrary the common belief that SRL instruction is more suitable for students with high ability (Peeters et al., 2016), this study’s findings indicated that MAs improved more than HAs. To the best of our knowledge, no intervention study has specifically been designed for weak students. Future studies should consider designing a tailor-made SRL intervention program for weak students, and examine how the degree of teacher support and student autonomy should be adjusted to help them develop their self-regulatory skills and confidence in learning gradually.

Finally, participant representativeness also limits the generalizability of this study as only one secondary school participated. Therefore, the performance and perceptions of HAs and MAs in this study are not representative of all HAs and MAs in Hong Kong. In addition, this study classified students into different achiever groups based solely on their school’s internal examination scores. Future studies should include larger and more representative samples of students, and use more objective classification criteria.

Conclusion

This study provides several new insights on SRL instruction. First, positive perceptions of the CC intervention program among both teachers and students support the cross-cultural applicability of SRL instruction in a subject area deeply rooted in Confucian culture. Specifically, teachers and students may be more likely to accept this instruction method as it facilitates learning, regardless of whether it is consistent with or contrary to their original approaches. Second, despite their important role in SRL instruction, student-related factors have seldom been investigated in previous intervention studies. This study’s findings showed that students’ baseline SRL levels and class performance significantly affected the level of support provided by teachers and how much autonomy they would allow during class. This highlights the importance of student factors when investigating SRL instruction implementation. Third, significant achievement-level differences between student groups suggest the importance of exploring the possible differential effects of SRL instruction among students with different backgrounds and characteristics rather than treating all students as a homogeneous group. This study’s use of qualitative methods to collect data provides a new perspective for addressing the debate on which achiever groups benefit most from SRL instruction. On the one hand, the better performance among HAs supports the view that they are most suited to high-SRL environments. On the other hand, MAs displayed more obvious improvements through SRL instruction than HAs did because of the former’s low baseline SRL. These findings suggest that all learners can benefit from SRL instruction, but the processes and mechanisms may vary between achiever groups.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because only the principal investigator of the study can access the dataset. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to KLL, dinkylau@cuhk.edu.hk.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Survey and Behavioral Research Ethics Committee, The Chinese University of Hong Kong. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

This work was supported by the Hong Kong Research Grants Council (RGC 14612517).

Conflict of interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ Each interview or observation code was composed of the research method (SI, student interview; TI, teacher interview; O, observation), class code (CA, Class A; CB, Class B), and interview or observation date. TA and TB represent the teachers of Classes A and B, respectively. HA and MA represent students from Classes A and B, respectively.
  2. ^ All lessons and interviews were conducted in Cantonese. The excerpts in this paper are verbatim translations.

References

Bai, B., Wang, J., and Nie, Y. (2021). Self-efficacy, task values and growth mindset: What has the most predictive power for primary school students’ self-regulated learning in English writing and writing competence in an Asian Confucian cultural context? Cambridge J. Educ. 51, 65–84. doi: 10.1080/0305764X.2020.1778639

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broadbent, J., and Poon, W. L. (2015). Self-regulated learning strategies and academic achievement in online higher education learning environments: A systematic review. Internet High. Educ. 27, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.iheduc.2015.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chi, L. C., and Chiou, G. F. (2015). Wenyanwen yuedu lijie lichen tanjiu [The comprehension process of reading Classic Chinese texts]. Huayuwen Jiaoxue Yanjiu 12, 51–74.

Google Scholar

Cousins, E., Bol, L., and Luo, T. (2022). Exploring long-term impacts of self-regulated learning interventions in K-12 contexts: A systematic review. Curr. Issues Educ. 23. doi: 10.14507/cie.vol23iss1.2013 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dignath, C., and Büttner, G. (2008). Components of fostering self-regulated learning among students: A meta-analysis on intervention studies at primary and secondary school level. Metacogn. Learn. 3, 231–264. doi: 10.1007/s11409-008-9029-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dignath, C., and Büttner, G. (2018). Teachers’ direct and indirect promotion of self-regulated learning in primary and secondary school mathematics classes – insights from video-based classroom observations and teacher interviews. Metacogn. Learn. 13, 127–157. doi: 10.1007/s11409-018-9181-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dignath, C., and Veenman, M. V. (2021). The role of direct strategy instruction and indirect activation of self-regulated learning—Evidence from classroom observation studies. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 33, 489–533. doi: 10.1007/s10648-020-09534-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donker, A. S., de Boer, H., Kostons, D., Van Ewijk, C. D., and van der Werf, M. P. (2014). Effectiveness of learning strategy instruction on academic performance: A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 11, 1–26. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2013.11.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dörrenbächer, L., and Perels, F. (2016). Self-regulated learning profiles in college students: Their relationship to achievement, personality, and the effectiveness of an intervention to foster self-regulated learning. Learn. Individ. Differ. 51, 229–241. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.09.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emagnaw, A. B. (2019). Self-regulated learning strategies and school performance in higher and lower students in secondary and preparatory school. J. Sch. Educ. Technol. 14, 37–48. doi: 10.26634/jsch.14.4.15914

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, J. A., Freed, R., and Sawyer, R. K. (2019). Fostering creative performance in art and design education via self-regulated learning. Instr. Sci. 47, 127–149. doi: 10.1007/s11251-018-9479-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, W., Lau, K. L., and Wei, J. (2019). Teacher feedback and students’ self-regulated learning in mathematics: A comparison between a high-achieving and a low-achieving secondary schools. Stud. Educ. Eval. 63, 48–58. doi: 10.1016/j.stueduc.2019.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hattie, J., Biggs, J., and Purdie, N. (1996). Effects of learning skills interventions on student learning: A meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 66, 99–136. doi: 10.3102/00346543066002099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heirweg, S., De Smul, M., Merchie, E., Devos, G., and Van Keer, H. (2020). Mine the process: Investigating the cyclical nature of upper primary school students’ self-regulated learning. Instr. Sci. 48, 337–369. doi: 10.1007/s11251-020-09519-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Housand, A., and Reis, S. M. (2008). Self-regulated learning in reading: Gifted pedagogy and instructional settings. J. Adv. Acad. 20, 108–136. doi: 10.4219/jaa-2008-865

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jayawardena, P. R., van Kraayenoord, C. E., and Carroll, A. (2019). Science teachers’ practices: Teaching for self-regulated learning in relation to Pintrich and Zusho’s (2007) model. Int. J. Educ. Res. 94, 100–112. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2018.09.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, K. L. (2004). Construction and initial validation of the Chinese Reading Motivation Questionnaire. Educ. Psychol. 24, 845–865. doi: 10.1080/0144341042000271773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, K. L. (2012). Instructional practices and self-regulated learning in Chinese language classes. Educ. Psychol. 32, 427–450.

Google Scholar

Lau, K. L. (2017). Classical Chinese reading instruction: Current practices and their relationship with students’ strategy use and reading motivation. Teach. Teach. Educ. 64, 175–186.

Google Scholar

Lau, K. L. (2018). Language skills in classical Chinese text comprehension. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 47, 139–157.

Google Scholar

Lau, K. L. (2019). Hong Kong senior secondary students’ reading motivation and Classical Chinese reading comprehension. Read. Write. 32, 963–982.

Google Scholar

Lau, K. L., and Chen, X. B. (2013). Perception of reading instruction and self-regulated learning: A comparison between Chinese students in Hong Kong and Beijing. Instruct. Sci. 41, 1083–1101.

Google Scholar

Lombaerts, K., Engels, N., and van Braak, J. (2009). Determinants to teachers’ recognitions of self-regulated learning practices in elementary education. J. Educ. Res. 102, 163–173. doi: 10.3200/JOER.102.3.163-174

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohammadi, R. R., Saeidi, M., and Ahangari, S. (2020). Self-regulated learning instruction and the relationships among self-regulation, reading comprehension and reading problem solving: PLS-SEM approach. Cogent Educ. 7:1746105. doi: 10.1080/2331186X.2020.1746105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mokhtari, K., and Reichard, C. A. (2002). Assessing students’ metacognitive awareness of reading strategies. J. Educ. Psychol. 94:249. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.2.249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neitzel, C., and Connor, L. (2017). Messages from the Milieu: Classroom instruction and context influences on elementary school students’ self-regulated learning behaviors. J. Res. Child. Educ. 31, 548–560. doi: 10.1080/02568543.2017.1347113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Otto, B., and Kistner, S. (2017). Is there a Matthew effect in self-regulated learning and mathematical strategy application? Assessing the effects of a training program with standardized learning diaries. Learn. Individ. Differ. 55, 75–86. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panadero, E. (2017). A review of self-regulated learning: Six models and four directions for research. Front. Psychol. 8:422. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panadero, E., Jonsson, A., and Botella, J. (2017). Effects of self-assessment on self-regulated learning and self-efficacy: Four meta-analyses. Educ. Res. Rev. 22, 74–98. doi: 10.1016/j.edurev.2017.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, J., De Backer, F., Kindekens, A., Triquet, K., and Lombaerts, K. (2016). Teacher differences in promoting students’ self-regulated learning: Exploring the role of student characteristics. Learn. Individ. Differ. 52, 88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2016.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, N. E. (1998). Young children’s self-regulated learning and contexts that support it. J. Educ. Psychol. 90, 715–729. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.90.4.715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, N. E., and Rahim, A. (2011). “Studying self-regulated learning in classroom,” in Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance, eds B. J. Zimmerman and D. H. Schunk (New York, NY: Routledge Press), 122–136.

Google Scholar

Perry, N. E., Hutchinson, L., and Thauberger, C. (2007). Mentoring student teachers to design and implement literacy tasks that support self-regulated reading and writing. Read. Writ. Q. 23, 27–50. doi: 10.1080/10573560600837636

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, N. E., VandeKamp, K. O., Mercer, L. K., and Nordby, C. J. (2002). Investigating teacher-student interactions that foster self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. 31, 5–15. doi: 10.1207/S15326985EP3701_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R., and Zusho, A. (2002). “The development of academic self-regulation: The role of cognitive and motivational factors,” in Development of achievement motivation, eds A. Wigfield and J. S. Eccles (San Diego: Academic Press), 249–284. doi: 10.1016/B978-012750053-9/50012-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pressley, M., and Afflerbach, P. (1995). Verbal protocols of reading: The nature of constructively responsive reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. doi: 10.2307/358808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulleyblank, E. G. (1995). Outline of Classical Chinese Grammar. Vancouver: UBC Press.

Google Scholar

Ren, M., and Yang, J. (2019). Regional monitoring of the reading ability for classical Chinese texts among disadvantaged eighth grade students. Educ. Meas. Assess. 2019, 40–49.

Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H., and Greene, J. A. (2018). “Historical, contemporary, and future perspectives on self-regulated learning and performance,” in Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance, 2nd Edn, eds D. H. Schunk and J. A. Greene (New York, NY: Routledge), 1–15. doi: 10.4324/9781315697048-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schunk, D. H., and Zimmerman, B. J. (1997). “Developing self-efficacious readers and writers: The role of social and self-regulatory processes,” in Reading engagement: Motivating readers through integrated instruction, eds J. T. Guthrie and A. Wigfield (Newark, DE: International Reading Association), 34–50.

Google Scholar

Shangguan, C. (2008). Gaozhong wenyanwen yuedu jiaohu jiaoxue shiyan yanjiu [The effectiveness of reciprocal teaching on classical Chinese reading]. Ph.D thesis, Jinhua: Zhejiang Normal University.

Google Scholar

Song, C. (2021). Wenyanwen yuedu jiaoxue de wenti yu gaijin cuoshi [Problems of classical Chinese reading instruction and suggestions for improvement]. Wenxue Jiaoyu 2021, 99–100.

Google Scholar

Stoeger, H., and Ziegler, A. (2010). Do pupils with differing cognitive abilities benefit similarly from a self-regulated learning training program? Gifted Educ. Int. 26, 110–123. doi: 10.1177/026142941002600113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stoeger, H., Fleischmann, S., and Obergriesser, S. (2015). Self-regulated learning (SRL) and the gifted learner in primary school: The theoretical basis and empirical findings on a research program dedicated to ensuring that all students learn to regulate their own learning. Asia Pac. Educ. Rev. 16, 257–267. doi: 10.1007/s12564-015-9376-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, Z. Y., and Sun, L. J. (2013). Gaozhong wenyanwen yuedu zhiliang mubiao yanjiu zhiyi: Beijing ji laiyuan fenxi [A study on classical Chinese reading objectives instruction in senior secondary grades: Background and source analysis]. Yuwen Jiaoxue Tongxun 10, 7–9.

Google Scholar

Usher, E. L., and Schunk, D. H. (2018). “Social cognitive theoretical perspective of self-regulation,” in Handbook of self-regulation of learning and performance, 2nd Edn, eds D. H. Schunk and J. A. Greene (New York, NY: Routledge), 19–35. doi: 10.4324/9781315697048-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Grinsven, L., and Tillema, H. (2006). Learning opportunities to support student self-regulation: Comparing different instructional formats. Educ. Res. 48, 77–91. doi: 10.1080/00131880500498495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Virtanen, P., and Nevgi, A. (2010). Disciplinary and gender differences among higher education students in self-regulated learning strategies. Educ. Psychol. 30, 323–347. doi: 10.1080/01443411003606391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L. (1979). Gudai hanyu changshi [Knowledge of classical Chinese]. Beijing: Renmin Jiaoyu Chubanshe.

Google Scholar

Wei, X. J. (2009). Chuzhong wenyanwen yuedu jiaoxue xianzhuang diaocha ji wenti pouxi [A survey and analysis on classical Chinese reading instruction in junior secondary grades]. Wenjiao Ziliao 2009, 135–136.

Google Scholar

Winne, P. H., and Perry, N. E. (2000). “Measuring self-regulated learning,” in Handbook of self-regulation, eds M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (San Diego: Academic Press), 531–566. doi: 10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50045-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xiong, J. P. (1993). Tantan zhongxue wenyanwen yuedu jiaoxue [Classical Chinese reading instruction in secondary schools]. Kecheng⋅Jiaocai⋅Jiaofa 6, 10–15.

Google Scholar

Yan, Z. (2018). How teachers’ beliefs and demographic variables impact on self-regulated learning instruction. Educ. Stud. 44, 564–577. doi: 10.1080/03055698.2017.1382331

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, S. L. (2005). Gudai hanyu jiaocheng [Course in classical Chinese]. Shanghai: Fudan Daxue Chubanshe.

Google Scholar

Zhao, G. Q. (2004). Tushi lilun yu wenyanwen yuedu jiaoxue [Schema theory and classical Chinese reading instruction]. Wenjiao Ziliao 2004, 115–119.

Google Scholar

Zhou, Z. S. (2007). Wenyanwen yuedu qianyi nengli de tigao [Enhancing classical Chinese reading ability]. Yuwen Jiaoxue Yu Yanjiu 14, 44–45.

Google Scholar

Zhu, J., and Mok, M. M. C. (2018). Predicting primary students’ self-regulated learning by their prior achievement, interest, personal best goal orientation and teacher feedback. Educ. Psychol. 38, 1106–1128. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2018.1497775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J. (2001). “Theories of self-regulated learning and academic achievement: An overview and analysis,” in Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives, eds B. J. Zimmerman and D. H. Schunk (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 1–38.

Google Scholar

Zohar, A., and Peled, B. (2008). The effects of explicit teaching of metastrategic knowledge on low-and high-achieving students. Learn. Instr. 18, 337–353. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: achievement-level differences, reading intervention, self-regulated learning instruction, Chinese students, teacher implementation

Citation: Lau KL (2022) Exploring achievement-level differences in implementing self-regulated learning instruction in a classical Chinese reading intervention program. Front. Educ. 7:948650. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2022.948650

Received: 20 May 2022; Accepted: 12 September 2022;
Published: 03 October 2022.

Edited by:

Zhengdong Gan, University of Macau, Macao SAR, China

Reviewed by:

Lan Yang, The Education University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
Yuehai Xiao, Hunan Normal University, China

Copyright © 2022 Lau. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kit Ling Lau, dinkylau@cuhk.edu.hk; orcid.org/0000-0002-6696-6524

Download